Welcome to Unmasking Why — where big questions meet clear answers, and sometimes, even stranger ones.
Let’s begin with something we take for granted: color.
We think we see colors all the time — the orange of a flame, the green of leaves, the shimmering blues of a peacock’s feathers. But here’s a twist that might change how you see… well, everything:
We Don’t See Colors — Not Directly
What our eyes actually detect is light — more specifically, wavelengths of light in the visible part of the electromagnetic radiation. These wavelengths bounce off objects and hit the retina at the back of your eye.
So where does “color” come in?
Your brain invents it.
It converts those raw wavelengths into experiences. A wavelength of ~480 nm? You call it “blue.” One near 700 nm? You call it “red.” But those labels — and even the experience of redness or blueness — are mental constructions.
How the Eye Translates Light into Color
Inside your retina are two kinds of photoreceptor cells: rods and cones.
- Rods help you see in dim light, but they don’t detect color.
- Cones, on the other hand, are color specialists.
There are three types of cones — sensitive roughly to:
- Short wavelengths (S-cones) → blue
- Medium wavelengths (M-cones) → green
- Long wavelengths (L-cones) → red
Color, then, is the result of your brain comparing how much each cone type is stimulated. You don’t have a “yellow” cone — yellow is just your brain interpreting a blend of red and green signals.
What About Color blindness?
In people with color vision deficiency (commonly known as colorblindness), one or more cone types don’t function normally.
- Red-green colorblindness (the most common) occurs when L-cones or M-cones are missing or malfunctioning.
- The result? Difficulty distinguishing between reds and greens — colors that, to most, are clearly different.
To a person with this kind of vision, the world is no less real, but it’s differently mapped. Their “reality” is tuned to another visual spectrum — one that might be hard for the rest of us to even imagine.
How Animals, Birds, and Bees See the World
Humans see a narrow band of the electromagnetic spectrum. But in nature, color perception is radically diverse:
- Bees can see ultraviolet (UV) light, which is invisible to us. Many flowers have UV patterns (“nectar guides”) that are like glowing signs to pollinators.
- Birds often have four types of cones (humans have three), including UV sensitivity. This gives them tetrachromatic vision, allowing them to perceive color dimensions we cannot fathom.
- Snakes can detect infrared, essentially “seeing” heat.
- Dogs have only two cone types — they see a more limited range of colors, often compared to red-green colorblind humans.
The way each species perceives the world isn’t just about survival — it’s about constructing a reality that fits their needs. Nature doesn’t have one truth — it has many interpretations.
Remember The Dress?
In 2015, the internet lost its mind over a seemingly simple question: What color is this dress?
Is it blue and black? Or white and gold?
When I looked at it, I saw white and gold. What do you see?
Some saw a blue-and-black dress, others saw white-and-gold — and arguments broke out in families and offices worldwide.
Why? Because the image lacked reliable contextual lighting cues. Some people’s brains assumed it was in shadow, others assumed it was brightly lit. And that tiny difference changed their perception of color completely.
If something as basic as the color of a dress can cause such perceptual disagreement, what does that say about objective reality?
So… What Is Reality?
If color is a mental construct — if your brain is painting over the world with meaning — then what’s real?
The apple isn’t red. It reflects certain wavelengths.
Your eye isn’t seeing color. It’s detecting light.
Your mind isn’t recording reality. It’s rebuilding it.
And everyone’s reconstruction might be a little different.
Who Gets to Define Reality?
If perception is filtered through biology and brain wiring, then… who’s to say what’s normal?
Is the person with colorblindness “wrong,” or just tuned to a different spectrum?
Is the bee’s world more colorful than ours?
Is the person who “hears” colors through synesthesia misperceiving, or hyper-perceiving?
Perhaps reality isn’t fixed, but filtered — shaped by both nature and nurture, perception and perspective.
Conclusion
What we call color — and even what we call real — is often a story told by our brain using raw sensory data and a generous helping of interpretation.
So next time you admire a brilliant sunset or a vivid flower, remember:
You’re not just seeing the world.
You’re translating it.
And that’s a wonder in itself.
How do you know that Bees ‘see’ UV light? Just because they have photoreceptors for UV light? Even so, the only way to tell would be to hit these ostensible ‘UV photoreceptors’ with UV light – and observe What? bee Optic nerve stimulation? How? as an effect? in a simulated experiment. Can you direct me to a peer-reviewed article or articles?
See “Mechanisms, functions and ecology of colour vision in the honeybee Review Open access Published: 15 May 2014”
Link “https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00359-014-0915-1”
“The ultraviolet colour component enhances the attractiveness of red flowers of a bee-pollinated plant”
Link “https://academic.oup.com/jpe/article/13/3/354/5843813”